Earworms: caracterizando una imaginación musical cotidiana

Autores/as

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.24215/18530494e073

Palabras clave:

earworm, imaginación musical involuntária, memoria musical, musicalidad

Resumen

Earworms, o Imaginación Musical Involuntaria (Involuntary Musical Imagery, InMI), constituyen una de las formas más difundidas de la imaginación musical, designando la experiencia mental espontánea con fragmentos musicales generalmente melódicos, familiares al individuo y de manera repetitiva. La terminología InMI es imprecisa, ya que puede englobar fenómenos diversos como obsesiones o alucinaciones, mientras que los earworms constituyen una experiencia más específica, cotidiana y no patológica, altamente prevalente, aunque comprendida de manera incipiente. Este trabajo tiene como objetivo discutir (1) la literatura actual sobre el fenómeno, identificando consensos emergentes y puntos controvertidos, (2) ciertas limitaciones metodológicas, y (3) las interpretaciones generalistas corrientes que asocian la experiencia a conceptos cognitivos correlatos. A partir de una revisión narrativa de las investigaciones teóricas y empíricas en los campos de la cognición musical, la psicología y las neurociencias, los resultados muestran que tratar el fenómeno como una mera rememoración involuntaria valida algunos predictores intuitivos, como la exposición musical repetitiva y los desencadenantes asociativos, pero oscurece matices mnemónicos específicos a la cognición musical. El artículo contribuye al entendimiento interdisciplinario de los earworms, ofreciendo conocimientos sobre su naturaleza como una manifestación cotidiana de la musicalidad humana, indicando la existencia de alguna especificidad cognitiva musical para la comprensión del fenómeno.

Descargas

Métricas

Visualizaciones del PDF
50
Dec 07 '24Dec 10 '24Dec 13 '24Dec 16 '24Dec 19 '24Dec 22 '24Dec 25 '24Dec 28 '24Dec 31 '24Jan 01 '255.0
|
Visualizaciones del HTML
16
Jan 2025Jul 2025Jan 20264.0
Visualizaciones de otros formatos
27
Jan 2025Jul 2025Jan 202611

Biografía del autor/a

Vitor Yamaguchi, Universidade Estadual Paulista, Brasil

Vitor Djun Yamaguchi es licenciado en Composición (beca FAPESP) por el Instituto de Artes de la UNESP y tiene una maestría en Cognición Musical (beca CAPES) por la misma institución, con investigaciones centradas en Memoria, Imaginación y Creatividad. Trabaja profesionalmente en las áreas de Música, Teatro y Audiovisual.

Citas

Arthur, C. (2023). Why do songs get “stuck in our heads”? Towards a theory for explaining earworms. Music & Science, 6, 20592043231164581. https://doi.org/10.1177/20592043231164581

Baddeley, A. D. y Hitch, G. (1993). The recency effect: Implicit learning with explicit retrieval? Memory & Cognition, 21(2), 146-155. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202726

Bailes, F. (2015). Music in mind? An experience sampling study of what and when, towards an understanding of why. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 58-68. https://doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000078

Beaman, C. P. (2018). The literary and recent scientific history of the earworm: A review and theoretical framework. Auditory Perception & Cognition, 1(1-2), 42-65. https://doi.org/10.1080/25742442.2018.1533735

Beaman, C. P., Powell, K. y Rapley, E. (2015). Want to block earworms from conscious awareness? B(u)y gum! Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(6), 1049-1057. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1034142

Beaman, C. P. y Williams, T. I. (2010). Earworms (stuck song syndrome): Towards a natural history of intrusive thoughts. British Journal of Psychology, 101(4), 637-653. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712609x479636

Beaman, C. P. y Williams, T. I. (2013). Individual differences in mental control predict involuntary musical imagery. Musicae Scientiae, 17(4), 398-409. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864913492530

Beaty, R. E., Burgin, C. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Kwapil, T. R., Hodges, D. A. y Silvia, P. J. (2013). Music to the inner ears: Exploring individual differences in musical imagery. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1163-1173. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.07.006

Berntsen, D. (1996). Involuntary autobiographical memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10(5), 435-454. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199610)10:5<435::AID-ACP408>3.0.CO;2-L

Berntsen, D. (2021). Involuntary autobiographical memories and their relation to other forms of spontaneous thoughts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Biological Sciences, 376(1817), 20190693. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0693

Berntsen, D. y Hall, N. M. (2004). The episodic nature of involuntary autobiographical memories. Memory & Cognition, 32(5), 789-803. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195869

Byron, T. P. y Fowles, L. C. (2015). Repetition and recency increase involuntary musical imagery of previously unfamiliar songs. Psychology of Music, 43(3), 375-389. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735613511506

Calvert, S. L. y Tart, M. (1993). Song versus verbal forms for very-long-term, long-term, and short-term verbatim recall. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 14(2), 245-260. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/0193-3973(93)90035-T

Campbell, S. M. y Margulis, E. H. (2015). Catching an earworm through movement. Journal of New Music Research, 44(4), 347-358. https://doi.org/10.1080/09298215.2015.1084331

Cotter, K. N., Christensen, A. P. y Silvia, P. J. (2016). Musical minds: Personality, schizotypy, and involuntary musical imagery. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 26(3), 220–225. https://doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000158

Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. Psychology Press.

Dawkins, R. (1976). Selfish gene. Oxford University Press.

Denmark, F. L. (2010). Zeigarnik effect. En I. B. Weiner y W. E. Craighead (Eds.), The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology (pp. 1873–1874). John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0924

Farrugia, N., Jakubowski, K., Cusack, R. y Stewart, L. (2015). Tunes stuck in your brain: The frequency and affective evaluation of involuntary musical imagery correlate with cortical structure. Consciousness and Cognition, 35, 66-77. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.04.020

Fitch, W. T. (2015). Four principles of bio-musicology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1664), 20140091. https://doi.org/doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0091

Floridou, G. A. y Müllensiefen, D. (2015). Environmental and mental conditions predicting the experience of involuntary musical imagery: An experience sampling method study. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 472-486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.02.012

Floridou, G. A., Williamson, V. J. y Emerson, L. M. (2018). Towards a new methodological approach: A novel paradigm for covertly inducing and sampling different forms of spontaneous cognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 65, 126-140. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.07.014

Floridou, G. A., Williamson, V. J. y Stewart, L. (2017). A novel indirect method for capturing involuntary musical imagery under varying cognitive load. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(11), 2189-2199. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1227860

Floridou, G. A., Williamson, V. J., Stewart, L. y Müllensiefen, D. (2015). The involuntary musical imagery scale (IMIS). Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25, 28-36. https://doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000067

Goodman, S. (2010). Sonic warfare: Sound, affect, and the ecology of fear. The MIT Press.

Halpern, A. R. y Bartlett, J. C. (2011). The persistence of musical memories: a descriptive study of earworms. Music Perception, 28(4), 425-432. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2011.28.4.425

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. y Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2-3), 61-83. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0999152x

Huron, D. (2001). Is music an evolutionary adaptation? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 43-61. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05724.x

Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. Bradford book.

Hyman, I. E., Cutshaw, K. I., Hall, C. M., Snyders, M. E., Masters, S. A., Au, V. S. K. y Graham, J. M. (2015). Involuntary to intrusive: Using involuntary musical imagery to explore individual differences and the nature of intrusive thoughts. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25, 14-27. https://doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000075

Hyman, I. E., Burland, N. K., Duskin, H. M., Cook, M. C., Roy, C. M., McGrath, J. C. y Roundhill, R. F. (2013). Going Gaga: Investigating, creating, and manipulating the song stuck in my head. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27(2), 204-215. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2897

Jakubowski, K. (2020). Musical imagery. En A. Abraham (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination (pp. 187-206). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108580298.013

Jakubowski, K., Bashir, Z., Farrugia, N. y Stewart, L. (2018). Involuntary and voluntary recall of musical memories: A comparison of temporal accuracy and emotional responses. Memory & Cognition, 46(5), 741-756. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0792-x

Jakubowski, K., Farrugia, N., Halpern, A. R., Sankarpandi, S. K. y Stewart, L. (2015). The speed of our mental soundtracks: Tracking the tempo of involuntary musical imagery in everyday life. Memory & Cognition, 43(8), 1229-1242. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0531-5

Jakubowski, K., Finkel, S., Stewart, L. y Müllensiefen, D. (2017). Dissecting an earworm: Melodic features and song popularity predict involuntary musical imagery. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11, 122-135. https://doi.org/10.1037%2Faca0000090

Jue, D., Jianping, M. y Yiduo, Y. (2020). Validation of the Chinese involuntary musical imagery scale and its application in mainland China. Musicae Scientiae, 26(2), 326-338. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864920948572

Killingly, C., Lacherez, P. y Meuter, R. (2021). Singing in the brain: investigating the cognitive basis of earworms. Music Perception, 38(5), 456-472. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2021.38.5.456

Kvavilashvili, L. y Mandler, G. (2004). Out of one's mind: a study of involuntary semantic memories. Cogn Psychol, 48(1), 47-94. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0285(03)00115-4

Liikkanen, L. A. (2012). Musical activities predispose to involuntary musical imagery. Psychology of Music, 40(2), 236-256. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735611406578

Liikkanen, L. A. (2012). Inducing involuntary musical imagery: An experimental study. Musicae Scientiae, 16(2), 217-234. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864912440770

Liikkanen, L. A. y Jakubowski, K. (2020). Involuntary musical imagery as a component of ordinary music cognition: A review of empirical evidence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(6), 1195-1217. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01750-7

Liikkanen, L. A., Jakubowski, K. y Toivanen, J. M. (2015). Catching earworms on Twitter: Using big data to study involuntary musical imagery. Music Perception, 33(2), 199-216. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2015.33.2.199

Mace, J. H. (2018). Involuntary autobiographical memories: Spontaneous recollections of the past. En K. Christoff y K. C. R. Fox (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming (pp. 469-476). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.37

Mace, J. H., Atkinson, E., Moeckel, C. H. y Torres, V. (2011). Accuracy and perspective in involuntary autobiographical memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(1), 20-28. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1634

MacLeod, C. M. (2020). Zeigarnik and von Restorff: The memory effects and the stories behind them. Memory & Cognition, 48(6), 1073-1088. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-020-01033-5

Margulis, E. H. (2014). On repeat: How music plays the mind. Oxford University Press.

McCrae, R. R. y John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 175-215. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x

Mills, C., Herrera-Bennett, A., Faber, M. y Christoff, K. (2018). Why the mind wanders: How spontaneous thought’s default variability may support episodic efficiency and semantic optimization. En K. Christoff y K. C. R. Fox (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming (pp. 11-22). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.42

Moeck, E. K., Hyman Jr, I. E. y Takarangi, M. K. T. (2018). Understanding the overlap between positive and negative involuntary cognitions using instrumental earworms. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 28(3), 164-177. https://doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000217

Moseley, P., Alderson-Day, B., Kumar, S. y Fernyhough, C. (2018). Musical hallucinations, musical imagery, and earworms: A new phenomenological survey. Consciousness and Cognition, 65, 83-94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.07.009

Müllensiefen, D., Fry, J., Jones, R., Jilka, S., Stewart, L. y Williamson, V. J. (2014). Individual differences predict patterns in spontaneous involuntary musical imagery. Music Perception, 31(4), 323-338. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2014.31.4.323

Negishi, K. y Sekiguchi, T. (2020). Individual traits that influence the frequency and emotional characteristics of involuntary musical imagery: An experience sampling study. PLoS ONE, 15(6), e0234111. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0234111

Pinker, S. (1998). Como a mente funciona. (2ª ed., L. T. Motta, Trad.). Companhia das Letras.

Pitman, M. M., Geffen, T. y Nettleton, P. (2021). Psychometric evaluation of the Involuntary Musical Imagery Scale (IMIS) in a south african sample. Musicae Scientiae, 27(2), 495-509. https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649211028643

Poe, E. A. (1845). The imp of the perverse. Random House.

Reyna, V. F. y Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 7(1), 1-75. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/1041-6080(95)90031-4

Smith, J. D., Wilson, M. y Reisberg, D. (1995). The role of subvocalization in auditory imagery. Neuropsychologia, 33(11), 1433-1454. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(95)00074-d

Snyder, B. (2000). Music and memory: An introduction. The MIT Press.

Sripada, C. S. (2018). An exploration/exploitation trade-off between mind-wandering and goal-directed thinking. En K. Christoff y K. C. R. Fox (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming (pp. 23-34). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.28

Szendy, P., Nancy, J.-L. y ProQuest. (2008). Listen: A history of our ears. Fordham University Press.

Talarico, J. M. y Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories. Psychological Science, 14(5), 455-461. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.02453

Twain, M. (1876-1878). Punch, brothers, punch! and other stories. Slote, Woodman & Co.

Varella, M. A. C. (2018). A evolução adaptativa das propensões artísticas. En M. E. Yamamoto y J. V. Valentova (Eds.), Manual de psicologia evolucionista (pp. 524–543). EDUFRN. https://repositorio.usp.br/item/002996659

Varella, M. A. C., Varella Valentova, J. y Fernández, A. M. (2015). Evolution of artistic and aesthetic propensities through female competitive ornamentation. En M. L. Fisher (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Women and Competition (pp. 757–784). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199376377.013.46

Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101(1), 34-52. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.34

Weir, G., Williamson, V. J. y Müllensiefen, D. (2015). Increased involuntary musical mental activity is not associated with more accurate voluntary musical imagery. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25, 48-57. https://doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000076

Williams, T. (2015). The classification of involuntary musical imagery: the case for earworms. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25, 5-13. https://doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000082

Williamson, V. J. y Jilka, S. R. (2014). Experiencing earworms: An interview study of involuntary musical imagery. Psychology of Music, 42(5), 653-670. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735613483848

Williamson, V. J., Jilka, S. R., Fry, J., Finkel, S., Müllensiefen, D. y Stewart, L. (2012). How do “earworms” start? Classifying the everyday circumstances of involuntary musical imagery. Psychology of Music, 40(3), 259-284. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735611418553

Williamson, V. J., Liikkanen, L. A., Jakubowski, K. y Stewart, L. (2014). Sticky tunes: How do people react to involuntary musical imagery? PLoS ONE, 9(1), e86170. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086170

Publicado

05-12-2024

Cómo citar

Yamaguchi, V. (2024). Earworms: caracterizando una imaginación musical cotidiana. Epistemus. Revista De Estudios En Música, Cognición Y Cultura, 12(2), 073. https://doi.org/10.24215/18530494e073

Número

Sección

Artículos originales de investigación